Skip to main content

Paired Comparisons Could Mean Better Witness Identifications

Compared with traditional lineup techniques, a series of two-faces-at-a-time choices led to more accurate identification by study witnesses.

Science, Quickly

In 2006 a 26-year-old California man named Uriah Courtney was sentenced to life in prison for kidnapping and rape, despite having an alibi for the time the crimes were committed. 

“And there were two witnesses. They saw a lineup in the police station, and they both identified the same person. And he was convicted, entirely based on those two eyewitness accounts.”

Salk Institute for Biological Studies neuroscientist Tom Albright. He says years later, the California Innocence Project looked into the case. 


On supporting science journalism

If you're enjoying this article, consider supporting our award-winning journalism by subscribing. By purchasing a subscription you are helping to ensure the future of impactful stories about the discoveries and ideas shaping our world today.


“And it turns out that the DNA that was found at the crime scene was not the DNA of Uriah Courtney.”

After eight years behind bars, Courtney was set free. But hiscase is not unique.

“There are now hundreds of cases in which individuals have been exonerated based on this post-conviction DNA analysis.”

Most of these innocent people were sent to prison because witnesses misidentified them.

“Somebody picked them out of a lineup, and that information was taken seriously by the police. And the jury believed it.”

Why do witnesses sometimes get it so wrong? Albright explains that our memory for visual events is notoriously flawed. 

“If somebody tells us that they saw something, we figure, well, it must be true. They saw it with their own eyes.”

Lineups typically show witnesses photos of six faces—five of innocent people and one of the suspect. 

“The eyewitness is simply asked to identify any person that they remember from the crime scene.”

But only having them pick their top choice doesn’t account for how well the witness remembers that face. This issue can result in errors.

Albright’s team thinks there’s a better way—by tapping into the strength of the witness’s memory. In an experiment, they had volunteers watch a clip of a grisly crime scene from an obscure Hollywood movie.    

The next day, these study subject “witnesses” viewed a six-person lineup that showed just two faces at a time. Think of an eye test: Better now—or now?

“So on each pair, the witness will vote for one or the other of the faces: Which one looks more similar to the person you remember from the crime scene? We then tabulate that vote. And the face that has the largest number of votes is the winner.”

Compared to traditional lineup techniques, the two-faces-at-a-time method led to a less biased and more accurate identification of the fictional perpetrator.

“People are far better at making relative judgments than they are at making absolute judgements.”

The study is in the journal Nature Communications. [Sergei Gepshtein et al., A perceptual scaling approach to eyewitness identification]

The researchers think their approach to lineups has the potential to reduce wrongful convictions, resulting in more justice for all.

—Susanne Bard

[The above text is a transcript of this podcast.]

Susanne Bard is a science writer and multimedia producer based on the West Coast. She has created content for Scientific American, Science magazine, and the American Association for the Advancement of Science, as well as for museums and zoos.

More by Susanne Bard
Paired Comparisons Could Mean Better Witness Identifications